Introduction
Hot carcass weights of pigs have been steadily increasing in the United States as the pork industry has been successful in their efforts to increase growth efficiency and improve genetic selection of lean-type pigs (Wu et al., 2017). These advancements have resulted in a trend for hot carcass weights to increase by an average of 0.59 kg/yr since 1995 (USDA-NASS, 2019). If this trend continues, it will impact hot carcass weight as well as the resulting size, weight, and the thickness of the resulting retail cuts from these carcasses. It is currently unknown what effect these changes will have on consumer acceptance and purchase intent of pork sold at retail.
The preferences consumers have when purchasing fresh meat are important to the meat industry, as consumers will not purchase a product that does not meet their expectations (Font-i-Furnols and Guerrero, 2014). Consumers are typically more willing to purchase fresh pork in a retail setting if the product has visual characteristics that they consider desirable (Dransfield et al., 2005). Historically, research has indicated that consumers rely heavily on color and marbling when purchasing fresh pork in a retail setting (Brewer et al., 2001; Norman et al., 2003), but it is unclear if these traits remain the major drivers when portion size is altered due to increased carcass weights. Within the beef industry, research has shown consumers more readily select thicker cut steaks compared to thinner cut steaks (Sweeter et al., 2005). Furthermore, in some instances, consumers find thickness, rather than price, to be the most important factor in fresh beef steak selection (Leick et al., 2012). As these previous studies indicate, consumers rely heavily on thickness when making purchasing decisions. Chop thickness will inevitably be impacted by increased pork carcass weight as the industry continues to utilize portion-controlled cutting for chops at retail. As carcass weights increase, it will result in thinner portion-controlled chops (Leick et al., 2011) and will ultimately impact consumer purchase decisions. However, there are no studies currently demonstrating how consumer purchase intent is affected by variability in chop size and thickness in fresh pork. Therefore, the objective of this study was to determine the impact of increased carcass weight and varying chop thicknesses on consumer preference and purchase intent of pork top loin chops.
Materials and Methods
The Kansas State University (KSU) Institutional Review Board approved the procedures used in this study (IRB 7440.4, Nov. 2017).
Loin collection, fabrication, and packaging
The pigs used in this study were intentionally raised to reach heavier live weights compared to today’s industry standard. Briefly, Lerner et al. (2018) described how 976 pigs were fed to reach heavier market weights to determine the impact of space allotment on growth performance. Pigs used in this experiment were originated from common genetic lines (PIC 327 boar × Camborough female) to be representative of the US commercial swine industry. At the commercial farm, pigs were allotted to 1 of 6 pen floor space or marketing treatments selected to elicit differences in final body weight. All pigs utilized for carcass and meat quality response criteria were marketed at the final barn marketing over 2 d. At the conclusion of the 160-d trial, pigs were transported to a commercial Midwest processor where harvest took place on 2 separate days over a 3-d period. At harvest, carcasses were sorted by hot carcass weight into a light group (LT; under 111.8 kg), medium light group (MLT; 111.8 to 119.1 kg), medium heavy group (MHVY; 119.1 to 124.4 kg), and heavy group (HVY; 119.1 to 124.4 kg). Twenty-five whole boneless pork loins (Institutional Meat Purchase Specification #413; North American Meat Institute, 2014) from each weight treatment group were selected from random carcasses on each harvest day (N = 200). They were then vacuum packaged and transported to the KSU Meat Laboratory and stored at 2 to 4°C until fabrication.
Loins were fabricated on Day 7, 8, or 9 postmortem (32 to 36 loins/d) the morning prior to consumer visual panels. Loins were cut immediately posterior to the M. spinalis dorsi and the posterior end of the loin was used for all analyses. Posterior loin sections were then fabricated from anterior to posterior with consecutively cut chops paired. Each pair was cut to 1 of 4 predetermined chop thicknesses (1.27, 1.91, 2.54, and 3.18 cm) using a cutting guide with the order of the thicknesses randomized for each loin. After fabrication, chops were individually weighed, and pressed on blotting paper (Whatman gel blotting paper, 46 × 57 cm, grade 601; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), with the blotted chop outline traced to later measure chop length, width, and loin eye area. Care was taken during blotting to ensure that the size of the chops were not distorted due to thickness. Chop length and width were measured at the widest and longest points on the chop outline (Fig. 1). Loin eye area was measured using a USDA grid with equally spaced dots measuring in 0.6 cm2, excluding accessory muscles surrounding the M. longissimus dorsi. Length, width, and loin eye area for each chop was measured by 2 different KSU team members and the values were averaged for each measurement.
One chop from each thickness pair was designated to labeled consumer visual analysis and the mirror chop was designated to unlabeled visual panels. Chops designated for unlabeled visual panels were individually placed on styrofoam trays (#17S, white; Dyne-a-Pak, Toronto, ON, Canada) with an absorbent pad. Chops designated for labeled panels were individually placed with an absorbent pad on larger styrofoam trays (#34, white; Dyna-Pak) to accommodate the label without covering the chop. All chops were then overwrapped with a PVC film (HIYG Gold Stretch Meat film, O2 transmission rate = 1191 cm3/0.065 m2/24 h, Berry Plastics Corporation, Evansville, IN). Additionally, for chops assigned to labeled visual panels, a KSU Meat Laboratory fresh meat product label containing cut identifications, package weight, package price/lb., and total price was placed on the right side of the package to avoid covering the chop (Fig. 2). Price per lb was determined by averaging prices at local grocery stores to obtain an average price ($4.52/lb) for the Manhattan, KS, area in the week prior to panels. Both labeled and unlabeled packages were labeled with an individual 4-digit code. Chops were held at 2 to 4°C until consumer panels were conducted.
Immediately prior to consumer visual evaluation, instrumental color readings and visual color and marbling scores were determined. Instrumental color values were assessed through the packaging using a Hunter Lab Miniscan spectrophotometer (Illuminant A, 2.54-cm aperture, 10° observer, Hunter Lab Associates Laboratory, Reston, VA). Visual color and marbling scores were assigned by a trained KSU team member according to the National Pork Producers Council pork quality standards (National Pork Producers Council, 1999). Additionally, chops were vacuum packaged immediately after consumer panels, aged to 10 d postmortem, and frozen at –40°C prior to further analyses (Rice et al., 2019).
Consumer visual panels
Panelists (N = 393) were recruited from Manhattan, KS and surrounding areas and paid for their participation. Panels were conducted in the KSU Color Laboratory. Panelists were provided an electronic tablet (Model 5709 HP Stream 7; Hewlett-Packard, Palo Alto, CA) with a digital survey (Version 2417833; Qualtrics Software, Provo, UT) to evaluate chops. Appearance and purchase intent were evaluated on continuous line scales with anchors at 0 (extremely undesirable/extremely unlikely to purchase), 50 (neither desirable or undesirable/would neither purchase or not purchase), and 100 (extremely desirable/extremely likely to purchase). Consumers were also asked to determine if each chop was desirable or undesirable (yes/no) overall and if they would or would not purchase each individual chop. If the consumer indicated they would not purchase a chop, they were then prompted to indicate a reason why: “color”, “firmness”, “chop size”, “chop thickness”, “marbling”, “external fat”, or “other”. For labeled chops, consumers were given additional options of price/lb, total package price, and total package weight. If the consumer chose “other”, they had the opportunity to type an open-ended response. Consumers could select more than one reason for a “no” response. Each panel consisted of 8 panelists. Both labeled and unlabeled chops were displayed in 2 separate coffin style cases (model DMF8; Tyler Refrigeration Corp., Niles, MI) at 2 to 4°C under fluorescent lights (32 W Del-Warm White 3000°K; Phillip Lighting Co., Somerset, NJ) which emitted an average intensity of 2230 ± 34 lx to replicate a retail experience. Panelists were first asked to fill out a demographics survey and, after further instructions, were taken to a retail case containing the 16 unlabeled packages (one from each weight treatment × chop thickness combination). The survey program randomly assigned the order each chop was viewed by each consumer. After completing the evaluation of the first case, consumers were prompted by the survey to proceed to the second case containing labeled packages with the paired chops from the unlabeled evaluations, and chops were evaluated using the same procedures described above. Panels were conducted over 6 d, with 8 to 9 panel sessions per day, with each panel session consisting of 8 panelists. Each panel lasted approximately 30 min and each consumer was only allowed to participate once.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed using the PROC GLIMMIX procedure of SAS (SAS Version 9.4; SAS Inst. Inc., Cary, NC). Data were analyzed as a split-plot design. The model included the whole plot factor of weight treatment and subplot factors of chop thickness, label type, and all interactions. For all acceptability data, a model with binomial error distribution was used. For all analyses, the Kenward-Roger approximation was used and α was set at 0.05. The PDIFF option was used to separate means when the overall treatment effect or effect of interactions were significant (P < 0.05). For interactions, the SLICE option was used to restrict comparisons to within a single factor.
Results
The effect of hot carcass weight and chop thickness on chop size
There was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05) for chop weight represented in Fig. 3. As chop thickness increased, the chops from all weight treatments became heavier (P < 0.05) compared to thinner chops. Additionally, within each thickness, chops from the HVY weight treatment were heavier (P < 0.05) than chops form the LT weight treatment. But as chops became thinner, fewer differences were found among the weight treatment groups, with no difference (P > 0.05) found among the 3 lightest weight treatments when chops were cut to 1.27 cm. The main effects of hot carcass weight and chop thickness for chop size measurements are reported in Table 1. As hot carcass weight increased, there was an increase (P < 0.05) in loin eye area, with chops from the HVY weight treatment being larger than all other weight categories, and chops from LT weight treatment being smaller (P < 0.05) than all other treatments, other than MLT. Additionally, chops from the HVY hot carcass weight treatment were longer (P < 0.05) than all other weight treatments, which were all similar (P > 0.05) in length. However, no differences (P > 0.05) were found among all weight treatments for chop width. There was also an increase (P < 0.05) in chop size due to chop thickness. Chops with a thickness of 2.54 and 3.18 cm had a greater (P < 0.05) loin eye area compared to chops with a thickness of 1.27 cm, with chops cut to a thickness of 1.91 cm being similar (P > 0.05) in loin eye area to all thicknesses. There were no differences (P > 0.05) in chop width among all chop thickness treatments. For chop length, chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 and 2.54 cm were similar (P > 0.05) with greater (P < 0.05) chop lengths than chops cut to thickness of 1.91 and 3.81 cm. Although there were statistical differences for loin eye area and chop length when chops were cut to different thicknesses, these differences were minimal (all treatments differing by no more than 2.2 cm2 and 0.3 cm).
Loin eye area, cm2 | Width, cm | Length, cm | |
Carcass weight1 | |||
LT | 72.4c | 7.3 | 12.0b |
MLT | 75.1bc | 7.4 | 12.0b |
MHVY | 76.2b | 7.5 | 12.2b |
HVY | 80.9a | 7.7 | 12.6a |
SEM2 | 1.17 | 0.10 | 0.10 |
P-value | < 0.01 | 0.07 | < 0.01 |
Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 74.9b | 7.5 | 12.3a |
1.91 | 75.9ab | 7.4 | 12.1b |
2.54 | 77.1a | 7.5 | 12.4a |
3.18 | 76.4a | 7.5 | 12.1b |
SEM2 | 0.71 | 0.09 | 0.06 |
P-value | < 0.01 | 0.84 | < 0.01 |
a–cLeast squares means within weight treatment or chop thickness differ (P < 0.05).
1Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and greater.
2SEM = (largest) of the least square means in the same section of the same column.
Chop color and marbling
There was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05; Table 2) for L* color readings. Chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm from the MLT carcasses had greater (P < 0.05) L* values (lighter) compared to chops from the MHVY carcasses. Additionally, chops cut to a thickness of 1.91 cm within the MLT carcasses had greater (P < 0.05) L* values compared to chops from all other hot carcass weight treatments. No differences (P > 0.05) were found among weight treatment groups when chops were cut to either 2.54 or 3.18 cm. There was a label type × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05; Table 3) for a* values (redness). With the exception of chops cut to a thickness of 2.54 cm, labeled chops had greater a* values compared to unlabeled chops.
Carcass weight2 | Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 1.91 | 2.54 | 3.18 | |
LT | 58.7ab | 58.1b | 58.4 | 58.4 |
MLT | 58.8a | 58.7a | 58.5 | 58.3 |
MHVY | 58.0b | 58.1b | 58.0 | 57.9 |
HVY | 58.6ab | 58.0b | 58.1 | 58.4 |
SEM3 | 0.50 | 0.50 | 0.50 | 0.50 |
P-value | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.08 | 0.06 |
a,bLeast squares means within a chop thickness differ (P < 0.05).
1L* = lightness; 0 = black and 100 = white.
2Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and greater.
3SEM = (largest) of the least square means in the same column.
Label type2 | Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 1.91 | 2.54 | 3.18 | |
Labeled | 18.7a | 17.7a | 17.5 | 17.5a |
Unlabeled | 18.2b | 17.5b | 17.4 | 17.3b |
SEM3 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.08 |
P-value | < 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.06 | < 0.01 |
a,bLeast squares means within a chop thickness differ (P < 0.05).
1a* = redness; –60 = green and 60 = red.
2Package label: labeled packages contained price and weight information on a label on the package and unlabeled packages did not contain a label.
3SEM = (largest) of the least square means in the same column.
There was also a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05; Table 4) for visual marbling scores. When chops were cut to a thickness of 2.54 cm, chops from the HVY hot carcass weight treatment group had greater (P < 0.05) subjective marbling scores compared to chops from both the LT and MHVY weight treatment groups, with chops from the MLT weight treatment being similar (P > 0.05) to all other weight treatments. When the chops were cut to a thickness of 3.18 cm, chops from the MLT and HVY weight treatments were similar (P > 0.05) for visual marbling scores and greater (P < 0.05) than chops from the LT and MHVY hot carcass weight groups. No differences (P > 0.05) were found among weight groups for visual color scores when chops were cut to 1.27 or 1.91 cm thicknesses.
Carcass weight1 | Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 1.91 | 2.54 | 3.18 | |
LT | 2.4 | 2.3 | 2.2b | 2.2b |
MLT | 2.4 | 2.5 | 2.5ab | 2.6a |
MHVY | 2.3 | 2.4 | 2.3b | 2.2b |
HVY | 2.5 | 2.5 | 2.6a | 2.6a |
SEM3 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.10 |
P-value | 0.20 | 0.10 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 |
a,bLeast squares means within a chop thickness differ (P < 0.05).
1Marbling score: 1 to 10, according to the National Pork Board marbling standards.
2Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and greater.
3SEM = (largest) of the least squares means in the same column.
The main effect for instrumental chop color and visual chop color and marbling are presented in Table 5. For instrumental color readings, no differences (P > 0.05) were found for both a* and b* color readings among hot carcass weight treatment groups. However, b* was affected by chop thickness as chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 also had greater (P < 0.05) b* (more yellow) readings compared to all other treatments. There were no differences (P > 0.05) between label types for L* values, but labeled chops possessed greater (P < 0.05) b* values compared to unlabeled chops.
Treatment | L*1 | a*2 | b*3 | Color4 | Marbling5 |
Carcass weight6 | |||||
LT | 17.7 | 16.1 | 4.0 | 2.2c | |
MLT | 17.6 | 16.0 | 4.1 | 2.5ab | |
MHVY | 17.8 | 16.0 | 4.1 | 2.3bc | |
HVY | 17.8 | 16.1 | 4.2 | 2.6a | |
SEM7 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 0.77 | 0.09 | |
P-value | 0.86 | 0.91 | 0.37 | 0.02 | |
Chop thickness, cm | |||||
1.27 | 16.4a | 4.0b | 2.4 | ||
1.91 | 15.9b | 4.1a | 2.4 | ||
2.54 | 15.9b | 4.1a | 2.4 | ||
3.18 | 15.9b | 4.1a | 2.4 | ||
SEM7 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.05 | ||
P-value | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | 0.11 | ||
Package label8 | |||||
Labeled | 58.3 | 16.2a | 4.2a | 2.4 | |
Unlabeled | 58.2 | 15.9b | 4.0b | 2.4 | |
SEM7 | 0.17 | 0.09 | 0.04 | 0.05 | |
P-value | 0.18 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | 0.89 |
a–cLeast squares means within carcass weight, chop thickness, or package label type differ (P < 0.05).
1L* (lightness; 0 = black and 100 = white).
2a* (redness; –60 = green and 60 = red).
3b* (yellowness; –60 blue and 60 = yellow).
4Color scale: 1 to 6, according to the National Pork Board Color Standards.
5Marbling score: 1 to 10, according to the National Pork Board Marbling Standards.
6Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and greater.
7SEM = (largest) of the least square means in the same section of the same column.
8Package label: labeled packages contained price and weight information on a label on the package and unlabeled packages did not contain a label.
There were no differences (P > 0.05) among hot carcass weight treatment groups for visual color scores. However, chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm had a lower (P < 0.05) visual color scores compared to all other treatments, which were similar (P > 0.05). Additionally, labeled chops had a greater (P < 0.05) visual color scores compared to unlabeled chops, while no differences (P > 0.05) were found between label types for visual marbling scores.
Consumer demographics
The data obtained from the demographics portion of the survey are summarized in Table 6. Of the 393 consumers who participated, over half (52%) were female, and a majority were Caucasian (82.4%). Additionally, 60.2% were between the ages of 20 to 39 yr and 29.6% were over the age of 40. A majority (53.0%) of consumers indicated they had obtained a college degree or had completed post-graduate work. Of these consumers, 90.7% ate pork from 1 to 5 times a week, and 82% preferred their pork cooked from medium to well done. Consumers indicated the most important palatability trait when consuming pork was flavor (42.9%).
Characteristic | Response | Percentage of consumers |
Gender | Male | 48.0 |
Female | 52.0 | |
Household size | 1 person | 15.1 |
2 people | 24.2 | |
3 people | 18.4 | |
4 people | 25.0 | |
5 people | 7.9 | |
6 people | 9.4 | |
Marital status | Married | 43.1 |
Single | 56.9 | |
Age | Under 20 | 10.2 |
20 to 29 | 46.9 | |
30 to 39 | 13.3 | |
40 to 49 | 12.8 | |
50 to 59 | 9.7 | |
Over 60 | 7.1 | |
Ethnic origin | African-American | 1.8 |
Asian | 4.1 | |
Caucasian/white | 82.4 | |
Hispanic | 5.1 | |
Mixed race | 4.1 | |
Native American | 0.5 | |
Other | 2.0 | |
Income | Under $25,000 | 26.1 |
$25,000 to $34,999 | 10.0 | |
$35,000 to $49,999 | 11.3 | |
$50,000 to $74,999 | 13.3 | |
$75,000 to $99,999 | 13.0 | |
$100,000 to $149,999 | 14.6 | |
$150,000 to $199,999 | 7.4 | |
> $199,999 | 4.1 | |
Education level | Did not graduate high school | 0.3 |
High school graduate | 9.2 | |
Some college/technical school | 37.5 | |
College graduate | 31.3 | |
Post college graduate | 21.7 | |
Most important palatability trait when consuming pork | Tenderness | 31.6 |
Juiciness | 25.4 | |
Flavor | 42.9 | |
Most important visual trait when purchasing fresh pork | Chop color | 30.3 |
Chop firmness | 2.3 | |
Chop size | 13.7 | |
Marbling | 9.7 | |
Price/kg | 32.8 | |
Total price | 9.9 | |
Other | 1.3 | |
Preferred degree of doneness when consuming pork | Rare | 1.4 |
Medium rare | 12.0 | |
Medium | 26.1 | |
Medium well | 28.1 | |
Well done | 27.8 | |
Very well done | 4.6 | |
Weekly pork consumption | 1 to 5 times | 90.7 |
6 to 10 times | 7.7 | |
11 or more times | 1.6 |
Additionally, consumers were asked what quality trait was most important to them when purchasing fresh pork in a retail setting. The greatest percentage (32.8%) of consumers indicated that price/lb was the most important purchasing motivator followed closely by color (30.3%), and chop size (13.7%).
Visual consumer ratings
Consumers were asked to indicate on a continuous line scale an overall appearance rating and their purchase intent for each sample (Table 7). Both appearance and purchase intent ratings were affected by hot carcass weight treatment. For both appearance and purchase intent ratings, chops from the HVY and MHVY hot carcass weight treatment groups were similar (P > 0.05; within 1.5 units) and had greater (P < 0.05) ratings than chops from the LT hot carcass weight treatment (>2 units different). Chops from the MLT hot carcass weight treatment were similar (P > 0.05) to both the MHVY and LT weight treatments (all differed by ≤ 2 units). As chop thickness increased, there was an increase (P < 0.05) in consumer appearance ratings, with chops cut to thicknesses of 2.54 and 3.18 cm being similar (P > 0.05) and having greater (P < 0.05) appearance ratings compared to all other thicknesses. Chops cut to thicknesses of 2.54 and 1.91 cm were similar (P > 0.05) and had greater (P < 0.05) purchase intent ratings compared to chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm. Chops cut to thicknesses of 1.91 and 3.18 cm were similar (P > 0.05) for consumer purchase intent ratings. Chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm had both the lowest (P < 0.05) consumer appearance ratings and consumer purchase intent ratings. There were no differences (P > 0.05) between label types for both appearance and purchase intent ratings.
Treatment | Appearance rating1 | Purchaseintent rating2 | Percentage that would purchase3 |
Carcass weight4 | |||
LT | 61.1c | 58.9c | 62.0 |
MLT | 62.1bc | 59.7bc | 63.7 |
MHVY | 63.1ab | 60.9ab | 65.9 |
HVY | 64.5a | 62.2a | 66.8 |
SEM5 | 0.90 | 0.10 | 0.80 |
P-value | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | 0.08 |
Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 54.8c | 51.9c | 45.9d |
1.91 | 64.1b | 63.2ab | 71.5b |
2.54 | 66.3a | 64.3a | 73.9a |
3.18 | 65.7a | 62.3b | 65.0c |
SEM5 | 0.80 | 0.91 | 0.77 |
P-value | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 |
Package label6 | |||
Labeled | 62.8 | 60.2 | 63.2b |
Unlabeled | 62.7 | 60.7 | 66.0a |
SEM5 | 0.74 | 0.84 | 0.66 |
P-value | 0.83 | 0.36 | < 0.01 |
a–cLeast squares means within the same main effect (carcass weight, chop thickness, and package label) differ (P < 0.05).
1Consumer appearance ratings: 0 = extremely undesirable; 100 = extremely desirable.
2Consumer purchase intent ratings: 0 = extremely unlikely to purchase; 100 = extremely likely to purchase the chop.
3Percentage of consumers who indicated “Yes” they would purchase the chop.
4Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and above.
5SEM = (largest) of the least squares means in the same section of the same column.
6Package label: labeled packages contained price and weight information on a label on the package and unlabeled packages did not contain a label.
In addition, consumers were asked to indicate “yes” or “no” on whether the appearance was desirable and if they would purchase the package. There was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05; Table 8) for the percentage of consumers who indicated “yes” the overall chop appearance was desirable. Within all weight treatments, the lowest (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers rated chops with a thickness of 1.27 cm as “yes” they were desirable overall (all ≤54%). Additionally, chops with a thickness of 3.18 cm had a lower (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers who indicated they were desirable compared 1.91 and 2.54 cm chops in both the MLT and MHVY weight treatments. For the percentage of consumers who indicated “yes” they would purchase, no differences (P > 0.05) were found among hot carcass weight treatments (62.0 to 66.8%). A greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers (73.9%) indicated “yes” they would purchase chops cut to a thickness of 2.54 cm compared to all other thicknesses, with the lowest (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers (45.9%) indicating “yes” they would purchase chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm. Additionally, a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated “yes” they would purchase chops in unlabeled packages compared to chops in labeled packages.
Chop thickness, cm | Carcass weight1 | |||
LT | MLT | MHVY | HVY | |
1.27 | 54.0c | 55.9c | 57.2b | 61.8c |
1.91 | 73.1a | 73.6a | 73.9a | 70.3b |
2.54 | 70.5ab | 73.5a | 73.6a | 78.5a |
3.18 | 65.8b | 66.4b | 71.6a | 69.7b |
SEM2 | 2.20 | 2.20 | 2.20 | 2.10 |
P-value | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 |
a–cLeast squares means within the same column differ (P < 0.05).
1Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and greater.
2SEM = (largest) of the least squares means in the same column.
If a consumer indicated “no” they would not purchase a certain chop, the survey would then prompt the consumer to give more information as to why they would not purchase. There was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction (P < 0.05) for the percentage of consumers who indicated “no” they would not purchase due to chop “size” presented in Table 9. A greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated “no” they would not purchase chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm due to chop “size” within the LT, MLT, and MHVY hot carcass weight treatments. No differences (P > 0.05) were found among chop thicknesses for the percentage of consumers who indicated “no” they would not purchase due to chop “size” within the HVY hot carcass weight treatment. There was also a chop thickness × label type interaction (P < 0.05; Table 10) for the percentage of consumers who indicated “no” they would not purchase due to “color”. For chops cut to thicknesses of 1.27 and 1.91 cm, a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated they would not purchase due to “color” for unlabeled chops. However, there was the opposite effect for chops with a thickness of 3.18 cm, with a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers that indicated “no” they would not purchase labeled chops due to “color”. No differences (P > 0.05) were found between label types for chops cut to a thickness of 2.54 cm.
Chop thickness, cm | Carcass weight1 | |||
LT | MLT | MHVY | HVY | |
1.27 | 22.9a | 19.0a | 20.0a | 12.2 |
1.91 | 16.4b | 12.4b | 9.8b | 7.6 |
2.54 | 8.6c | 14.3ab | 8.1b | 10.7 |
3.18 | 10.6c | 9.8b | 10.7b | 11.8 |
SEM2 | 2.60 | 2.68 | 2.31 | 2.47 |
P-value | < 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.07 |
a–cLeast squares means within hot carcass weight differ (P < 0.05).
1Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and above.
2SEM = (largest) of the least squares means in the same column.
Label type1 | Chop thickness, cm | |||
1.27 | 1.91 | 2.54 | 3.18 | |
Labeled | 7.8b | 19.4b | 19.6 | 13.6a |
Unlabeled | 12.8a | 25.8a | 23.1 | 8.6b |
SEM2 | 1.24 | 2.23 | 2.29 | 1.42 |
P-value | < 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.23 | < 0.01 |
a–cLeast squares means in the same column differ (P < 0.05).
1Package label: labeled packages contained price and weight information on a label on the package and unlabeled packages did not contain a label.
2SEM = (largest) of the least squares means in the same column.
The main effect data for the reasons stated by consumers for not intending to purchase are presented in Table 11. There were no differences (P > 0.05) among hot carcass weight treatments for the percentage of consumers who indicated “no” they would not purchase due to chop “firmness”, “marbling”, “thickness”, “external fat”, “shape”, “purge”, “price/kg”, “total package weight”, “total package price”, or “other”.
Treatment | Firmness | Marbling | Thickness | External fat | Purge | Price/kg1 | Package weight, kg | Package price | Other |
Carcass weight2 | |||||||||
LT | 2.1 | 20.4 | 30.7 | 1.3 | < 0.1 | 2.8 | 1.1 | 1.4 | 2.8 |
MLT | 2.8 | 20.7 | 28.5 | 1.6 | < 0.1 | 2.6 | 1.3 | 2.0 | 1.9 |
MHVY | 2.6 | 22.1 | 33.5 | 1.9 | 0.3 | 3.4 | 2.2 | 2.9 | 1.8 |
HVY | 3.1 | 21.6 | 36.3 | 1.9 | < 0.0 | 4.4 | 1.5 | 1.7 | 1.5 |
SEM3 | 0.64 | 1.36 | 1.07 | 0.54 | 0.13 | 0.70 | 0.67 | 0.67 | 0.62 |
P-value | 0.72 | 0.93 | 0.09 | 0.84 | 0.48 | 0.39 | 0.63 | 0.38 | 0.52 |
Chop thickness, cm | |||||||||
1.27 | 1.6b | 13.7b | 48.7a | < 0.1 | < 0.1 | 2.1 | 2.1ab | 0.2c | 1.0c |
1.91 | 4.1a | 30.4a | 16.5c | 1.6 | < 0.1 | 3.1 | < 0.0b | 1.6bc | 2.9a |
2.54 | 2.8ab | 28.6a | 22.6b | 2.3 | < 0.1 | 3.8 | < 0.0b | 2.3ab | 1.5bc |
3.18 | 2.6ab | 15.8b | 48.1a | 1.8 | 0.3 | 4.6 | 2.5a | 3.7a | 2.6ab |
SEM3 | 0.71 | 1.15 | 0.99 | 0.48 | 0.13 | 0.98 | 0.58 | 0.67 | 0.55 |
P-value | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | 0.23 | 0.52 | 0.06 | 0.04 | < 0.01 | 0.02 |
Package label4 | |||||||||
Labeled | 2.3 | 20.1 | 33.0 | 1.3 | 0.2 | 2.1 | |||
Unlabeled | 3.0 | 22.3 | 31.3 | 2.0 | < 0.1 | 1.9 | |||
SEM3 | 0.41 | 0.96 | 0.65 | 0.35 | 0.09 | 0.40 | |||
P-value | 0.14 | 0.29 | 0.08 | 0.16 | 0.19 | 0.61 |
a–cLeast squares means in the same main effect (carcass weight, chop thickness, and package label) in the same column differ (P < 0.05).
1Price for each package at $4.52/lb.
2Carcass weight groups: LT = under 111.8 kg, MLT = 111.8 to 119.1 kg, MHVY = 119.1 to 124.4 kg, and HVY = 124.4 kg and above.
3SEM = (largest) of the least square means in the same section of the same column.
4Package label: labeled packages contained price and weight information on a label on the package and unlabeled packages did not contain a label.
Chop thickness did impact the reason consumers indicated they would not purchase chops. A greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated “no” they would not purchase chops cut to a thickness of 1.91 cm compared to 1.27 cm thick chops due to “firmness”. However, chops cut to a thickness of 2.54 and 3.18 cm were similar (P > 0.05) to all other thicknesses for the percentage of consumers who would not purchase due to “firmness”. For “marbling”, chops with a thickness of 1.91 and 2.54 cm were similar (P > 0.05), with a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers who indicated they would not purchase due to “marbling” compared to chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 and 3.18 cm. Among chop thickness groups, chops cut to the thicknesses of 1.27 and 3.18 cm were similar (P > 0.05), with the greatest (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers who indicated they would not purchase due to chop “thickness”. Additionally, chops cut to a thickness of 1.91 cm had the lowest (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers who indicated they would not purchase due to chop “thickness”. For “total package weight”, a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated they would not purchase chops cut to a thickness of 3.18 cm compared to chops cut to both 1.91 and 2.54 cm. For “total package price”, a greater (P < 0.05) percentage of consumers indicated they would not purchase chops cut to a thickness of 3.18 cm compared to chops cut to both 1.91 and 1.27 cm. There were no differences (P > 0.05) found between label types for the percentage of consumers who indicated “no” they would not purchase a chop for chop “firmness”, “marbling”, “thickness”, “external fat”, “purge”, and “other”.
Discussion
As hot carcass weights in the US pork industry increase, there should be an expected increase in the size of the retail cuts that come from these carcasses. This relationship between hot carcass weight and yield was demonstrated in pork by Cisneros et al. (1996) who observed an increase in the weight of boneless trimmed cuts as slaughter weight increased. Additionally, other studies in beef have yielded similar results (Abraham et al., 1980; Leick et al., 2011). Many different factors can affect consumer purchasing decisions. Although there are many sub-factors, visual sensory characteristics such as lean color, marbling, cut size and thickness, in addition to price have been shown to drive consumer purchasing decisions (Font-i-Furnols and Guerrero, 2014). In a study by Leick et al. (2012) that evaluated ribeye, sirloin, and top loin beef steaks of varying hot carcass weights and steak thicknesses, the authors asked consumers to rank factors such as color, marbling, steak thickness, price, and texture for steaks. For all cuts, they reported consumers ranked visual factors such as color, marbling, and steak thickness to be more important when making purchasing decisions than price (Leick et al., 2012). Additionally, consumers placed a great deal of value on the steak thickness, indicating that thickness was the most important purchasing factors when selecting sirloin steaks (Leick et al., 2012). Fewer studies have evaluated consumer purchasing motivators for pork, but the published work has suggested consumers rely heavily on color and marbling when making their purchasing decisions (Brewer et al., 2001; Norman et al., 2003). Therefore, it is important that as carcass weights increase in the US swine industry, the subsequent increase in size of chops will not lead to negative effects on the quality traits of color and marbling. It is also important that consumers find the resulting increase in the size of retail cuts acceptable.
The effect of hot carcass weight and thickness on chop size measurements
It is well documented that as hot carcass weight increases, there is a subsequent increase in the size of retail cuts (Abraham et al., 1980; Cisneros et al., 1996; Leick et al., 2011). Ultimately this size increase can result in thinner chops within a portion-control cutting setting (Dunn et al., 2000). As expected, in this study there was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction for chop weight. As chop thickness increased, the chops from all weight treatments became heavier compared to chops from the thinner thicknesses. Cisneros et al. (1996), evaluated the effect of pig slaughter weight, sex, and breed type on yield. They reported as slaughter weight increased, there was also an increase in loin weight; however loin eye area was not compared across weight treatments. Similar results were reported by Leick et al. (2011), who used portion-controlled cutting on beef carcasses from different hot carcass weight treatments and reported there was an increase in longissimus muscle area as hot carcass weights increased. Additionally, those authors also reported that as longissimus muscle area increased, there was a decrease in steak thickness due to portion-controlled cutting to specified weights (Leick et al., 2011). In the current work, the use of portion-controlled cutting was not possible due to the lack of access to the proper equipment. Thus, the different thicknesses used were chosen to best represent this reduction in chop thickness commonly seen with heavier cuts with larger loin eyes associated with weight-based portion-controlled cutting.
The effect of hot carcass weight, chop thickness, and label type on chop color and marbling
The pork industry uses lean color and marbling informally to determine potential eating quality. There have been conflicting results in the studies that have assessed both instrumental color and subjective color and marbling scores of pork carcasses with increasing hot carcass weights. Studies such as Park and Lee (2011) and Durkin et al. (2012), have assessed the impact of increased hot carcass weight on color and observed no differences in L* values. In the current study, as weight increased, there was a hot carcass weight × chop thickness interaction for L* color readings. These differences were very small and do not reflect the other studies that reported greater differences in L* values due to increased weight in pigs (Virgili et al., 2003; Latorre et al., 2004; Harsh et al., 2017).
There was also label type × chop thickness interaction for a* values (redness). With the exception of chops cut to a thickness of 2.54 cm, labeled chops had greater a* values compared to unlabeled chops. Though these differences were minor, this interaction may be explained by the methods in our study. Although all chops were allowed an adequate amount of time for oxygenation (bloom) before color readings were taken (greater than 1 h), color readings were measured on all unlabeled chops before labeled chops, during the time period when the labels were being applied to the labeled treatment. We believe that the extra time needed to label packages could have allowed labeled chops a greater amount of time to bloom and possibly could explain the observed differences in a* value, as well as the label type difference main effect for b* and visual color scores. Previous work has indicated that an a* value change of greater than 0.589 is needed for consumers to detect a difference in redness (Zhu and Brewer, 1999), indicating that the differences in the current study related to labeling type would not be detectable by consumers. Another study that utilized a scanning spectrophotometer measured color differences of chicken breast samples cut to 0.5, 1.0, and 1.5 cm and reported that differences in thickness could affect L*, a*, and b* values (Sandusky and Heath, 1996), though the authors offered no potential cause. Although the current study used pork, our results are consistent with Sandusky and Heath (1996) for a* and b* values. Moreover, there were no differences detected among hot carcass weight treatment groups for visual color, but there were differences in visual color scores between chop thicknesses; however, this difference was very small, with all treatments within 0.1 units.
In addition to color, marbling has been found to be an important visual cue for consumers when purchasing fresh pork in a retail setting (Fernandez et al., 1999; Brewer et al., 2001; Rincker et al., 2008). Although there were differences found for visual marbling scores in the current study, these differences did not favor either lighter or heavier hot carcass weights. Additionally, the observed differences in marbling were very small, with at most a 0.4 unit difference between any 2 weight treatments. This is further supported by the lack of difference found among the weight treatments when evaluated for the chemical percentage of intramuscular fat content, with all weight treatments differing by no more than 0.2% (Rice et al., 2019). These results are contradictory to similar studies that have reported increased marbling scores as hot carcass weight increased (Cisneros et al., 1996; Huff-Lonergan et al., 2002; Park and Lee, 2011; Harsh et al., 2017).
The effect of hot carcass weight and chop thickness on visual consumer ratings
Visual sensory factors are a cornerstone for the purchasing decisions consumers make at the fresh meat retail case. These visual factors include lean color, marbling, chop size, and cut thickness (Font-i-Furnols and Guerrero, 2014). As hot carcass weights increased, there were significant differences in some of these visual factors that consumers were able to detect in the current study. When consumers were asked to evaluate the overall appearance of chops and purchase intent on a line scale, for both appearance and purchase intent, consumers gave higher ratings as hot carcass weight treatment increased, indicating that consumers found the chops from the heavier carcasses to be more appealing overall and had a greater intent to purchase them. It is noteworthy that the reported differences, though different, were small in magnitude (<3.5 units). This is contradictory to a study by Sweeter et al. (2005) that assessed how increased ribeye area in beef steaks affected consumer purchasing decisions in a retail store by determining how long it took for each steak to be purchased at a local grocery store. They reported that there were no differences in the amount of time the steaks of different sizes stayed on the shelf (Sweeter et al., 2005). The authors concluded that hot carcass weight did not impact consumer purchasing decisions, unlike the current study where consumers preferred chops from heavier carcasses. Perhaps this difference between the current work and the study by Sweeter et al. (2005) is due to the related size difference in longissimus muscle area between pork and beef. It is plausible that the larger longissimus muscle in beef from heavier weight carcasses surpasses a consumer threshold for tolerance for ribeye size; whereas with the smaller longissimus muscle in pork, there is a greater relative increase in size that will be tolerated by consumers and an acceptance threshold for size that was not surpassed in the current study with the carcass weight ranges evaluated.
In addition to hot carcass weight, chop thickness impacted consumer ratings. Consumers found thicker chops to be more appealing; however, they were more willing to purchase chops with a thickness of 2.54 cm. This indicates that consumers prefer chops that are thicker, and is consistent with similar studies conducted in beef. Leick et al. (2011) asked consumers participating in their visual study with portion-controlled cut beef steaks from different hot carcass treatments, which visual trait was most important when purchasing beef steaks in a retail setting. For both top loin and sirloin steaks, consumers indicated that cut thickness was the most important trait, and the authors hypothesized that consumers felt they were getting more for their money with thicker cut steaks, even though the steaks were all cut to the same weight (Leick et al., 2011). In a follow-up, Leick et al. (2012) performed a similar study but added price as a factor. Again, when consumers were asked to rank the most important factors when purchasing beef steaks, and for all 3 cuts, color, marbling, and thickness were all ranked higher than price, and thus indicated consumers placed a greater importance on visual cues, and less on price. Additionally, they reported that consumers in the greater household income brackets selected a greater percentage of the least expensive ribeye steaks in comparison to consumers in the lower income brackets (Leick et al., 2012). This led the authors to conclude that a factor other than price impacted consumer selections of ribeye steaks. The results of the current study also reflect this, as the impact of carcass weight and chop thickness on the consumer purchase intent ratings was not dependent on knowing the price of the product (labeled vs. unlabeled), indicating consumers assessed the merit of these factors independently when determining their likelihood to purchase the product.
In addition to the appearance and purchase intent, consumers in the current study were asked a “yes” or “no” question on if the chop’s overall appearance was desirable, and if they would purchase the chop. Within all hot carcass weight treatment groups, chops cut to a thickness of 1.27 cm had the lowest percentage of consumers who indicated “yes” they were desirable. Similarly, in beef, Maples et al. (2018) used a digital survey to assess how beef steak thickness impacted consumer purchasing decisions and reported that a majority of consumers disliked thinner steaks. Although hot carcass weight did not impact the percentage of consumer who indicated “yes” they would purchase a chop, chop thickness was affected. A greater percentage of consumers indicated “yes” they would purchase chops from the middle thicknesses (1.91 and 2.54 cm), compared to chops cut both the thinnest or the thickest thickness. This indicates that when making a purchasing decision, although consumers like the appearance of the thicker chops, they can be cut too thick as well as too thin for the consumers to actually purchase them. Additionally, when asked if they would or wouldn’t purchase a chop, a greater percentage of consumer indicated “yes” they would purchase samples that were unlabeled. This could indicate that consumers were more willing to purchase chops that had no pricing information and demonstrating that price could play a greater role in consumer purchasing decisions when purchasing fresh pork what is indicated by previous authors with beef. It is noteworthy that the methods used in the current work relied on the consumers’ responses as to whether or not they would purchase the chops instead of using a victory auction or other methods commonly used to measure purchase intent where the consumers would have actually purchased the products. Therefore, the results of the current study should be interpreted as such, as it is unknown how the results related to the consumers’ purchase intent would have been impacted if they would have been required to actually purchase the products as opposed to simply indicating their willingness to purchase them.
If a consumer in the current study indicated “no” they would not purchase a chop, the survey would then prompt the consumer to give more information as to why they would not purchase. Of particular note, the percentage of consumers within each weight treatment who indicated they would not purchase a chop due to “size”, was dependent on chop thickness. For all of the carcass weight groups other than the HVY group, the greatest percentage of chops identified as a “no” for purchase due to “size” was the thinnest (1.27 cm) group. But, within the HVY treatment, thickness had no impact. In the current study, “size” was intentionally undefined as a point to not prompt consumers to think about and notice differences in and consider loin eye area when evaluating chops in the case, unless that was a common consideration for the individual consumer. Thus, in our study “size” is likely a reflection of both loin eye size as well as thickness. This is reflected as in the lighter weight treatments, the thinner chops had a greater percentage of samples that would not have been purchased due to “size”, whereas as in the HVY weight treatment, even the thinnest chops met consumer acceptance levels for “size”. These results also indicate that while in the current study we were able to produce chops that were too small, we were not able to produce chops that were too large for consumers.
However, thickness alone had a large impact on consumer intent to purchase. Both the thinnest (1.27 cm) and the thickest (3.18 cm) cut chops had the greatest percentage of consumers who indicated they would not purchase the chops due to their thickness. In the study by Leick et al. (2011), consumers gave greater ratings to thicker cut steaks, and the authors hypothesized it was due to consumers thinking they were getting more compared to thinner steaks with a greater eye area, despite all steaks being cut to the same weight (Leick et al., 2011). In the current work, both the thinnest (1.27 cm) and the thickest (3.18 cm) cut chops had fewer consumers indicate they would purchase them compared to the middle 2 thicknesses, further indicating that chops can be cut both too thin and too thick for consumer preferences. This is further supported by the consumers who indicated they would not purchase the chops due to “package weight”. As chop thickness increased, there was an increase in package weight which subsequently increased the package price. A greater proportion of consumers indicated they would not purchase the thickest and thinnest chops due to “package weight”, indicating that some of the packages were both too heavy and too light for consumers. This increase in package weight resulted in a concurrent increase in the package price. As chop thickness increased, a greater percentage of consumers indicated “no” they would not purchase due to package price. Therefore, as both package weight and price increased, it negatively impacted purchasing decisions. Though these results from the visual portion of the current study do not necessarily indicate price as the most important factor considered by consumers when purchasing pork, they do indicate that price is a consideration, and for some consumers, will be the primary reason for not purchasing the product of heavier weights. Similar results have been shown in beef. In a victory-auction scenario, Platter et al. (2005) reported consumers were willing to pay more for higher quality steaks, but the number of bids by consumers was greater for the less expensive steaks, providing evidence of the importance of price and the level of consumer-to-consumer variation in price tolerance for consumer purchasing decisions. It is noteworthy that 42.7% of consumers in the current study self-reported in the demographics survey that price was the most important factor when purchasing pork. However, results from the visual evaluation would indicate that other factors, specifically chop thickness, are as, if not more important drivers of consumer purchasing intent than price. These results are consistent with previous work who have indicated that other visual factors, including thickness, can be more important to consumer purchasing intent than price alone (Savell et al., 1989, Leick et al., 2012).
Overall, carcass weight, chop thickness, and label type affected consumer overall desirability and purchase intent for fresh pork. Consumers indicated that chops from heavier carcasses and thicker chops were more desirable. Additionally, consumers were more likely to purchase chops with a thickness of 2.54 cm, indicating that chops could become too thick as well as too thin for acceptance. As pork carcasses continue to become heavier with increased industry efficiencies, the resulting cuts will also increase in size. Results from the current work indicate that despite the associated increases in chop size, consumer desirability and purchase intent will not be negatively impacted.
Notes
- Funding, wholly or in part, was provided by The National Pork Board. [^]
- Contribution no 20-009-J of the Kansas Agricultural Experiment Station, Manhattan, KS 66506, USA [^]
Literature Cited
Abraham, H. C.Murphey, C. E.Cross, H. R.Smith, G. C.Franks, W. J. 1980. Factors affecting beef carcass cutability: An evaluation of the USDA yield grades for beef. J. Anim. Sci. 50:841–851. doi:10.2527/jas1980.505841xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas1980.505841x
Brewer, M. S.Zhu, L. G.McKeith, F. K.. 2001. Marbling effects on quality characteristics of pork loin chops: Consumer purchase intent, visual and sensory characteristics. Meat Sci. 59:153–163. doi:10.1016/S0309-1740(01)00065-1http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0309-1740(01)00065-1
Cisneros, F.Ellis, M.McKeith, F. K.McCaw, J.Fernando, R. L.. 1996. Influence of slaughter weight on growth and carcass characteristics, commercial cutting and curing yields, and meat quality of barrows and gilts from two genotypes. J. Anim. Sci. 74:925–933. doi:10.2527/1996.745925xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/1996.745925x
Dransfield, E.Ngapo, T. M.Nielsen, N. A.Bredahl, L.Sjödén, P. O.Magnusson, M.Campo, M. M.Nute, G. R.. 2005. Consumer choice and suggested price for pork as influenced by its appearance, taste and information concerning country of origin and organic pig production. Meat Sci. 69:61–70. doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2004.06.006http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2004.06.006
Dunn, J. L.Williams, S. E.Tatum, J. D.Bertrand, J. K.Pringle, T. D.. 2000. Identification of optimal ranges in ribeye area for portion cutting of beef steaks. J. Anim. Sci. 78:966–975. doi:10.2527/2000.784966xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2000.784966x
Durkin, I.Dadic, M.Brkic, D.Lukic, B.Kusec, G.Mikolin, M.Jerkovic, I.. 2012. Influence of gender and slaughter weight on meat quality traits of heavy pigs. Acta Agric 3:211–214.
Fernandez, X.Monin, G.Talmant, A.Mourot, J.Lebret, B.. 1999. Influence of intramuscular fat content on the quality of pig meat— 2. Consumer acceptability of m. longissimus lumborum. Meat Sci. 53:67–72. doi:10.1016/S0309-1740(99)00038-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0309-1740(99)00038-8
Font-i-Furnols, M.Guerrero, L.. 2014. Consumer preference, behavior and perception about meat and meat products: An overview. Meat Sci. 98:361–371. doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.06.025http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2014.06.025http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000341466900007&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
Harsh, B. N.Arkfeld, E. K.Mohrhauser, D. A.King, D. A.Wheeler, T. L.Dilger, A. C.Shackelford, S. D.Boler, D. D.. 2017. Effect of hot carcass weight on loin, ham, and belly quality from pigs sourced from a commercial processing facility. J. Anim. Sci. 95:4958–4970. doi:10.2527/jas2017.1674http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas2017.1674http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000417113700023&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
Huff-Lonergan, E.Baas, T. J.Malek, M.Dekkers, J. C. M.Prusa, K.Rothschild, M. F.. 2002. Correlations among selected pork quality traits. J. Anim. Sci. 80:617–627. doi:10.2527/2002.803617xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2002.803617x
Latorre, M. A.Lázaro, R.Valencia, D. G.Medel, P.Mateos, G. G.. 2004. The effects of gender and slaughter weight on the growth performance, carcass traits, and meat quality characteristics of heavy pigs. J. Anim. Sci. 82:526–533. doi:10.2527/2004.822526xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2004.822526x
Leick, C. M.Behrends, J. M.Schmidt, T. B.Schilling, M. W.. 2011. Consumer selection of constant-weight ribeye, top loin, and sirloin steaks. Meat Sci. 87:66–72. doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2010.09.004http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2010.09.004http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000284434600011&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
Leick, C. M.Behrends, J. M.Schmidt, T. B.Schilling, M. W.. 2012. Impact of price and thickness on consumer selection of ribeye, sirloin, and top loin steaks. Meat Sci. 91:8–13. doi:10.1016/j.meatsci.2011.11.021http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.meatsci.2011.11.021http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000300866300002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
Lerner, A. B.Tokach, M. D.DeRouchey, J. M.Dritz, S. S.Goodband, R. D.O’Quinn, T. G.Gonzales, J. M.Rice, E. A.Allerson, M. W.Price, H. E.Woodworth, J. C.. 2018. Effects of space allowance and marketing strategy on growth performance of pigs raised to heavy market weights. Kansas Agricultural Experiment Station Research Report 4. Kansas State Research and Extension, Manhattan. doi:10.4148/2378-5977.7687
Maples, J. G.Lusk, J. L.Peel, D. S.. 2018. Unintended consequences of the quest for increased efficiency in beef cattle: When bigger isn’t better. Food Policy 74:65–73. doi:10.1016/j.foodpol.2017.11.005http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodpol.2017.11.005http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000426412400006&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
National Pork Producers Council. 1999. Official color and marbling standards. NPPC, Des Moines, IA.
Norman, J. L.Berg, E. P.Heymann, H.Lorenzen, C. L.. 2003. Pork loin color relative to sensory and instrumental tenderness and consumer acceptance. Meat Sci. 65:927–933. doi:10.1016/S0309-1740(02)00310-8http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0309-1740(02)00310-8
North American Meat Institute. 2014. The meat buyer’s guide. 8th ed. North American Meat Institute, Washington, DC.
Park, B.Lee, C.. 2011. Feasibility of increasing the slaughter weight of finishing pigs. J. Anim. Sci. Technol. 53:211–222. doi:10.5187/JAST.2011.53.3.211http://dx.doi.org/10.5187/JAST.2011.53.3.211
Platter, W. J.Tatum, J. D.Belk, K. E.Koontz, S. R.Chapman, P. L.Smith, G. C.. 2005. Effects of marbling and shear force on consumers’ willingness to pay for beef strip loin steaks. J. Anim. Sci. 83:890–899. doi:/2005.834890xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2005.834890x
Rice, E. A.Lerner, A. B.Olson, B. A.Prill, L. L.Drey, L. N.Price, H. E.Lowell, J. E.Harsh, B. N.Barkley, K. E.Honegger, L. T.Richardson, E.Woodworth, J. C.Gonzalez, J. M.Tokach, M. D.DeRouchey, J. M.Dritz, S. S.Goodband, R. D.Allerson, M. W.Fields, B.Shackelford, S. D.King, D. A.Wheeler, T. L.Dilger, A. C.Boler, D. D.O’Quinn, T. G.. 2019. Effects of increased pork hot carcass weights. II: Loin quality characteristics and palatability ratings. Meat Musc. Biol. 3:447-456. doi:10.22175/mmb2019.07.0027
Rincker, P. J.Killefer, J.Ellis, M.Brewer, M. S.McKeith, F. K.. 2008. Intramuscular fat content has little influence on the eating quality of fresh pork loin chops. J. Anim. Sci. 86:730–737. doi:10.2527/jas.2007-0490http://dx.doi.org/10.2527/jas.2007-0490http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=Agronomy_sub&KeyUT=WOS:000253443900023&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=WOS_CPL&UsrCustomerID=9992b2403adf8c36119d0b6fce39b97c
Sandusky, C. L.Heath, J. L.. 1996. Effect of background color, sample thickness, and illuminant on the measurement of broiler meat color. Poult. Sci. 75:1437–1442. doi:10.3382/ps.0751437http://dx.doi.org/10.3382/ps.0751437
Savell, J. W.Cross, H. R.Francis, J. J.Wise, J. W.Hale, D. S.Wilkes, D. L.Smith, G. C.. 1989. National consumer retail beef study: Interaction of trim level, price and grade on consumer acceptance of beef steaks and roasts. J. Food Qual. 12:251–274. doi:10.1111/j.1745-4557.1989.tb00328.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4557.1989.tb00328.x
Sweeter, K. K.Wulf, D. M.Maddock, R. J.. 2005. Determining the optimum beef longissimus muscle size for retail consumers. J. Anim. Sci. 83:2598–2604. doi:10.2527/2005.83112598xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2005.83112598x
USDA-NASS. 2019. Federally inspected hog slaughter average dressed weight by month and year, US. (accessed 7 July 2019)https://www.nass.usda.gov/Charts_and_Maps/Livestock_Slaughter/hgdrwgx8.php.
Virgili, R.Degni, M.Schivazappa, C.Faeti, V.Poletti, E.Marchetto, G.Pacchioli, M. T.Mordenti, A.. 2003. Effect of age at slaughter on carcass traits and meat quality of Italian heavy pigs. J. Anim. Sci. 81:2448–2456. doi:10.2527/2003.81102448xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.2527/2003.81102448x
Wu, F.Vierck, K. R.DeRouchey, J. M.O’Quinn, T. G.Tokach, M. D.Goodband, R. D.Dritz, S. S.Woodworth, J. C.. 2017. A review of heavy weight market pigs: Status of knowledge and future needs assessment. Transl. Anim. Sci. 1:1–15. doi:10.2527/tas2016.0004
Zhu, L. G.Brewer, M. S.. 1999. Relationship between instrumental and visual color in a raw, fresh beef and chicken model system. J. Muscle Foods 10:131–146. doi:10.1111/j.1745-4573.1999.tb00391.xhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-4573.1999.tb00391.x